The power of a dog’s mind
How much do dogs understand about their owners? (Credit: Image Source/Alamy)
How much do dogs understand about their owners? (Credit: Image Source/Alamy)

(BBC) It is strange enough that people can get better when given pills that do not contain any actual medicine. Even more peculiarly, the same may be true of animals
The placebo effect is a strange and still not entirely understood quirk of medicine. On the face of it, someone’s health should not improve if they are given a simple sugar pill instead of a tablet carrying a drug. But sometimes it does.

Dogs do not necessarily understand what vets do (Credit: Grossemy Vanessa/Alamy)
Dogs do not necessarily understand what vets do (Credit: Grossemy Vanessa/Alamy)

It is just about possible to imagine that the placebo effect stems from a strong faith in the power of doctors to cure us. If that is the case, then the effect would be limited to our species – after all, animals do not really put their faith in veterinarians when they are sick. And, indeed, many scientists assume placebo effects are not seen in animals.
But not all scientists do. Some say there is evidence that placebo effects can and do play out in our pets.

It is not actually that uncommon to use placebos in animals. When a new animal therapy is on trial, scientists often test it using a similar procedure for evaluating human therapies. They give some animals the therapy and others a placebo, so they can fully assess the potency of a new therapy.

“Initially we sought out to use the placebo just as a baseline,” says Karen Muñana, a neurologist at the North Carolina State University College of Veterinary Medicine in the US, who studies epilepsy in dogs.
“But then in the evaluation of that data we realised that the dogs that had been on placebo had shown improvement in their seizure frequency compared to before we started the study,” she says. “Their seizure frequency went down.”

The data suggested that the dogs on placebo treatments were doing better than they had been doing before the trial. Their condition was improving even in the absence of medicine. They seemed to be displaying a placebo effect.
Why would this be the case? Muñana thinks that several factors could be at play.
One of the most obvious – but often overlooked – factors is that disease naturally changes in an animal over time.

“Epilepsy is a waxing and waning disease with a natural course,” says Muñana. “What happens in epilepsy is that owners will seek care when their dog’s disease – when their seizures in this case – are at the worst.”
This means it is possible that the dogs in Muñana’s epilepsy trial were experiencing particularly intense symptoms as the trial began. Even if they had not been put in the trial, their condition might have improved naturally over the course of a few weeks. If this is the explanation for the trial results, then the dogs were not really experiencing a placebo effect.
But this is not the only possible explanation. Just the effect of knowing that their animal is taking part in a drug trial can change the behaviour of a dog owner, says Muñana.

“The owner’s perception [is] that the dog is being monitored better, watched better,” she says. “Perhaps because they’re in the study [the owner is] more likely to give those medications that are being used to treat the underlying disease.”
Again, this would imply the animal itself is not experiencing a placebo effect. If anything, it is the pet owner who might be responding to the placebo. The owner who anxiously monitors their pet throughout its time in the trial might in fact be helping the pet get better. It is certainly something that can affect the animal’s convalescence that is not down to the drug being tested in the trial.

What this shows is that it is tricky to pin down a real placebo effect in animals, Muñana says.
One way to reduce some of these complicating factors would be to introduce a third group to animal drug trials. This group would receive absolutely no treatment, not even a placebo. This is sometimes known as a “waiting list” group. If the dogs on the placebo do better than the dogs on the waiting list, that might hint of a real placebo effect in animals.

Tricky study
But this is a tricky kind of study to carry out.
Trial participants are hard to come by and the dog owners who do enrol their pet in a study are doing so precisely because they want to take action to make their pet feel better. They might be reluctant to enrol in a study where the chance that their dog is receiving the trial therapy is lower than the chance that it is not.

“You want the owners to feel like they’re part of the study and [that their dog] may be getting the active drug,” says Muñana.
There are other ways an animal owner’s behaviour might have an impact. For instance, it might subtly alter the owner’s perception of the animal in a way that changes the animal’s behaviour.

Ellen Kienzle is a veterinary researcher who specialises in nutrition and dietetics at the Ludwig Maximilians University of Munich in Germany. She has been interested in the psychology of feeding animals for many years. “It’s one thing to have scientific knowledge, and another is getting to the animal. And there is always the owner in between,” she says.
Kienzle started hearing a lot of stories about certain feed supplements and their purportedly wondrous effects in horses. She wanted to get to the bottom of these anecdotes.
“I got fed up with all these supplements,” she says. She could see no solid scientific reason why, say, giving an adult horse a selenium supplement – even if it did not have a selenium deficiency – would help its muscle health.

Despite this, many riders Kienzle heard about were convinced that selenium helped horses with stiff muscles. “It was an ideal model,” she says. She designed a study to put two supplements to the test: selenium and vitamin E, alongside a placebo control.
“We found enormous placebo effects,” she says. But once again, that was not necessarily because the animals were responding directly to the placebo.
“Especially with horse riding, there is tremendous interaction between the rider and the horse,” she says. Kienzle compares riding to dancing. If your dancing partner suddenly spies someone they dislike, you would be able to feel the change in tension in their body.
You would know something is up, she says – and that might put you on edge too. “That is how, to my way of thinking, part of the communication between a horse and the rider works.”

The horse responds to subtle, even subconscious cues from the rider, like a sensitive dance partner might. “If your mood is good and you think, ‘Ah, today he or she is going to perform very well,’ the horse will fulfil your expectations. And also, if you expect the horse to make a certain error and that picture is in your mind, your body communicates exactly that to the horse.”
The power of this effect can be extraordinarily strong. It is not just about perceptions, but marked changes in behaviour.

SHARE THIS ARTICLE :
Facebook
Twitter
WhatsApp

Leave a Comment

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

All our printed editions are available online
emblem3
Subscribe to the Guyana Chronicle.
Sign up to receive news and updates.
We respect your privacy.